Interesting People mailing list archives

The Cognitive Biases Tricking Your Brain


From: "Dave Farber" <farber () gmail com>
Date: Mon, 6 Aug 2018 10:12:17 +0900



Begin forwarded message:

From: Dewayne Hendricks <dewayne () warpspeed com>
Subject: [Dewayne-Net] The Cognitive Biases Tricking Your Brain
Date: August 6, 2018 6:36:20 JST
To: Multiple recipients of Dewayne-Net <dewayne-net () warpspeed com>
Reply-To: dewayne-net () warpspeed com

The Cognitive Biases Tricking Your Brain
Science suggests we’re hardwired to delude ourselves. Can we do anything about it?
By BEN YAGODA
Sep 2018 Issue
<https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2018/09/cognitive-bias/565775/>

I am staring at a photograph of myself that shows me 20 years older than I am now. I have not stepped into the 
twilight zone. Rather, I am trying to rid myself of some measure of my present bias, which is the tendency people 
have, when considering a trade-off between two future moments, to more heavily weight the one closer to the present. 
A great many academic studies have shown this bias—also known as hyperbolic discounting—to be robust and persistent.

Most of them have focused on money. When asked whether they would prefer to have, say, $150 today or $180 in one 
month, people tend to choose the $150. Giving up a 20 percent return on investment is a bad move—which is easy to 
recognize when the question is thrust away from the present. Asked whether they would take $150 a year from now or 
$180 in 13 months, people are overwhelmingly willing to wait an extra month for the extra $30.

Present bias shows up not just in experiments, of course, but in the real world. Especially in the United States, 
people egregiously undersave for retirement—even when they make enough money to not spend their whole paycheck on 
expenses, and even when they work for a company that will kick in additional funds to retirement plans when they 
contribute.

That state of affairs led a scholar named Hal Hershfield to play around with photographs. Hershfield is a marketing 
professor at UCLA whose research starts from the idea that people are “estranged” from their future self. As a 
result, he explained in a 2011 paper, “saving is like a choice between spending money today or giving it to a 
stranger years from now.” The paper described an attempt by Hershfield and several colleagues to modify that state of 
mind in their students. They had the students observe, for a minute or so, virtual-reality avatars showing what they 
would look like at age 70. Then they asked the students what they would do if they unexpectedly came into $1,000. The 
students who had looked their older self in the eye said they would put an average of $172 into a retirement account. 
That’s more than double the amount that would have been invested by members of the control group, who were willing to 
sock away an average of only $80.

I am already old—in my early 60s, if you must know—so Hershfield furnished me not only with an image of myself in my 
80s (complete with age spots, an exorbitantly asymmetrical face, and wrinkles as deep as a Manhattan pothole) but 
also with an image of my daughter as she’ll look decades from now. What this did, he explained, was make me ask 
myself, How will I feel toward the end of my life if my offspring are not taken care of?

When people hear the word bias, many if not most will think of either racial prejudice or news organizations that 
slant their coverage to favor one political position over another. Present bias, by contrast, is an example of 
cognitive bias—the collection of faulty ways of thinking that is apparently hardwired into the human brain. The 
collection is large. Wikipedia’s “List of cognitive biases” contains 185 entries, from actor-observer bias (“the 
tendency for explanations of other individuals’ behaviors to overemphasize the influence of their personality and 
underemphasize the influence of their situation … and for explanations of one’s own behaviors to do the opposite”) to 
the Zeigarnik effect (“uncompleted or interrupted tasks are remembered better than completed ones”).

Some of the 185 are dubious or trivial. The ikea effect, for instance, is defined as “the tendency for people to 
place a disproportionately high value on objects that they partially assembled themselves.” And others closely 
resemble one another to the point of redundancy. But a solid group of 100 or so biases has been repeatedly shown to 
exist, and can make a hash of our lives.

The gambler’s fallacy makes us absolutely certain that, if a coin has landed heads up five times in a row, it’s more 
likely to land tails up the sixth time. In fact, the odds are still 50-50. Optimism bias leads us to consistently 
underestimate the costs and the duration of basically every project we undertake. Availability bias makes us think 
that, say, traveling by plane is more dangerous than traveling by car. (Images of plane crashes are more vivid and 
dramatic in our memory and imagination, and hence more available to our consciousness.)

[snip]




Dewayne-Net RSS Feed: http://dewaynenet.wordpress.com/feed/
Twitter: https://twitter.com/wa8dzp






-------------------------------------------
Archives: https://www.listbox.com/member/archive/247/=now
Modify Your Subscription: https://www.listbox.com/member/?member_id=18849915
Unsubscribe Now: 
https://www.listbox.com/unsubscribe/?member_id=18849915&id_secret=18849915-a538de84&post_id=20180805211226:C5E1077C-9915-11E8-8283-AA04D44F7F8D
Powered by Listbox: https://www.listbox.com

Current thread: